Showing posts with label Grw. Show all posts
Showing posts with label Grw. Show all posts

Thursday, April 03, 2025

Research Byte: A Systematic #Review of Theoretical Frameworks in #Reading and #Writing: Insights from JAAL (2015–2024)—#Grw #CHC #schoolpsychology #education #SPED




 

A Systematic Review of Theoretical Frameworks in Reading and Writing: Insights from JAAL (2015–2024) 

Click here to visit journal web page.  Not an open access article.

Abstract

This systematic review explored the theoretical frameworks used in reading and writing research published in the Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy (JAAL) between 2015 and 2024. Using the matrix method, we screened 126 articles for inclusion criteria and identified 20 studies that addressed both reading and writing in empirical research. We coded each article to determine which theories were explicitly or implicitly used, focusing on five frameworks frequently cited in literacy studies: sociocultural theory, reader response theory, motivation theory, social constructivism, and transactional theory. Results revealed sociocultural theory as the dominant lens, whereas other theories were mainly used as secondary frameworks and often cited implicitly. We also found limited integration across theories despite the acknowledged interdependence of reading and writing. These findings indicate a need for greater theoretical clarity and suggest that researchers and practitioners adopt more integrated approaches when investigating or teaching literacy. By highlighting the current state of theoretical application in JAAL, this review underscores opportunities to strengthen both research design and classroom practice through more explicit and multifaceted theoretical grounding.

Friday, January 31, 2025

Research Byte: Using Decoding Measures to Identify #ReadingDifficulties: A #Metaanalysis on English as a First Language Learners and English Language Learners #ELL #EL1


Using Decoding Measures to Identify Reading Difficulties: A Meta-analysis on English as a First Language Learners and English Language Learners

Click here to view at Journal of Educational Psychology

Students with or at risk of reading difficulties (RD) benefit from accurate early identification and intervention. Previous research has employed various decoding measures to screen students for RD, but the criteria for identification have been inconsistent. Assessing students with RD is especially challenging in English Language Learners (ELLs), as vocabulary deficits can impact decoding. Additionally, few research syntheses have examined whether researchers use different measures to screen ELLs and EL1s for RD, and whether these differences result in distinct decoding profiles between ELLs with RD and EL1s with RD. To address these gaps, this study uses a meta-analysis to examine the decoding measures used in RD assessments and whether outcomes differ for ELLs and EL1s. The findings show that real word reading assessments identify students with more pronounced decoding deficits than nonword reading assessments. Despite the use of different RD screening measures for ELLs and EL1s, the gap between ELLs with and without RD was similar to that between EL1s with and without RD. These results suggest that real word-reliant measures, which are influenced by word knowledge, provide a more comprehensive assessment of RD than nonword-reliant measures for both ELLs and EL1s. We encourage future researchers to use consistent decoding measures when screening RD in both populations, to maximize comparability of findings.

Friday, May 25, 2018

The Role of Executive Functions in Reading Comprehension - excellent overview of major models of reading comprehension

The Role of Executive Functions in Reading Comprehension. Article or link.

Reese Butterfuss and Panayiota Kendeou

ABSTRACT

Our goal in this paper is to understand the extent to which, and under what conditions, executive functions (EFs) play a role in reading comprehension processes. We begin with a brief review of core components of EF (inhibition, shifting, and updating) and reading comprehension. We then discuss the status of EFs in process models of reading comprehension. Next, we review and synthesize empirical evidence in the extant literature for the involvement of core components of EF in reading comprehension processes under different reading conditions and across different populations. In conclusion, we propose that EFs may help explain complex interactions between the reader, the text, and the discourse situation, and call for both existing and future models of reading comprehension to include EFs as explicit components.

Keywords Executive functions . Reading comprehension . Discourse processes


This article includes an excellent summary of the major models of reading comprehension. This post includes that select material.

The Status of Executive Functions in Models of Reading Comprehension

Reading comprehension is one of the most complex and important cognitive activities humans perform (Kendeou et al. 2016). Given its importance and complexity, researchers have sought to understand reading comprehension via the development and specification of a multitude of models and frameworks that account for various processes and mechanisms of reading.

Generally, reading comprehension refers to the construction of a mental representation of what the text is about (Kintsch and V an Dijk 1978). Although most models of reading comprehension converge on this general idea, the processes and assumptions by which readers construct such representations differ across models and frameworks. It is also important to note that a unified, comprehensive model of reading comprehension has yet to be established. McNamara and Magliano (2009) reviewed and compared one set of models, which are concerned primarily with the construction of the mental representation during reading: The Construction-Integration Model (Kintsch 1988), the Structure-Building Framework (Gernsbacher 1991), the Resonance Model (Albrecht and O'Brien 1993), the Event-Indexing Model (Zwaan et al. 1995), the Causal Network Model (Trabasso et al. 1989), the Constructionist Theory (Graesser et al. 1994), and the Landscape Model (van den Broek et al. 1999). In this review, we investigate the status of EFs in each of these models.

Among this set of models, the Construction-Integration (CI) model (Kintsch 1988) is perhaps the most comprehensive, and it is considered the best approximation to a true theory of reading comprehension (Kendeou and O'Brien 2017). According to the CI model, comprehension is the result of two processes, construction and integration. Construction refers to the activation of information in the text and background knowledge. There are four potential sources of activation: the current text input, the prior sentence, background knowledge, and prior text. As this information is activated, it is connected into a network of concepts. Integration refers to the continuous spread of activation within this network until activation settles. Activation sources from the construction process are iteratively integrated, and only those concepts that are connected to many others are maintained in the network. At the completion of reading, the result is a complete network or a mental representation of what the text is about. This mental representation has been termed the situation model. Even though the initial model makes no explicit reference to EFs, in a subsequent revision, Kintsch (1998) included a suppression mechanism in the CI model by adopting inhibitory links. Specifically, the CI model relies on links between information units to promote an appropriate representation of a text and inhibit inappropriate representations. In this context, facilitatory links connect related information units, and inhibitory (or negative) links connect conflicting or inappropriate information units. Inhibitory links serve to suppress or inhibit inappropriate representations (Kintsch 1998).

The Structure-Building Framework (Gernsbacher 1991) describes comprehension as the result of three processes. The first process, laying a foundation, involves using initial information from a text to lay the groundwork for a mental representation to be constructed. The second process, mapping, involves mapping information from the text onto that foundation to create structures. The third process, shifting, involves a shift to begin building a new structure when readers are unable to map information onto an existing structure. Irrelevant information that does not cohere with a current structure is suppressed. Thus, within the Structure-Building Framework, the suppression mechanism attempts to account for individual differences in comprehension ability. Specifically, the model posits that if incoming information is related to the current structure, then activation of that information is enhanced, resulting in its incorporation into the current structure. When information is not related to the current structure, then activation to that information is suppressed, or, alternatively, readers may shift and use that information to begin building a new structure. The suppression mechanism is the result of readers' ability to inhibit irrelevant information. This ability moderates reading comprehension in that skilled readers have a strong suppression mechanism and can therefore suppress irrelevant information, whereas less-skilled readers lack a strong suppression mechanism. As a result, less-skilled comprehenders' poor suppression ability may lead them to shift too often, which impairs comprehension because more information is competing for limited resources.

The Resonance Model (Myers and O'Brien 1998) attempts to account for factors that influence the activation of information during comprehension, particularly information that is no longer active in working memory. The model emphasizes automatic, memory-based retrieval mechanisms as fundamental assumptions. Specifically, the model assumes that information in working memory serves as a signal to all of memory, which activates information that resonates with the signal. Elements resonate as a function of the number of features that overlap with the contents of working memory. Even though the model has not explicitly incorporated any EFs, O'Brien et al. (1995) found that suppression was involved in processes relevant to the Resonance Model. Specifically, O'Brien et al. found that when an anaphoric phrase reactivated more than one potential antecedent from the text, the selected target antecedent was strengthened in long-term memory, whereas potential, but non-target, antecedents that interfered with the target antecedent were suppressed.

The Event-Indexing Model (Zwaan et al. 1995) was developed as an attempt to account more fully for processes involved with situation model construction of narrative texts. It operates under the assumption that readers monitor and establish coherence along five dimensions of continuity, and thus situation model construction: time, space, causality, motivation, and agents. Thus, within the event-indexing model, EFs such as shifting attention from one dimension to another as well as updating the construction of the situation model account for individual differences in comprehension ability. For example, Bohn-Gettler et al. (2011) found that there are developmental differences in children's ability to monitor the shifts in each of these dimensions.

The Causal Network Model (Trabasso et al. 1989) accounts for how readers generate causal inferences and represent causality during reading. Causal inferences are at the core of building a coherent representation of a story. Narrative elements can be categorized as either settings, events, goals, attempts, outcomes, or reactions. Also, there are assumed to be four types of causal relations: enabling, psychological, motivational, and physical. The model also provided a discourse analysis tool, Causal Network Analysis, to identify the causal structure that underlies story constituents. Overall, the model accounts for the importance of causal relations in memory for the text, but makes no assumptions about specific EFs. The Constructionist theory (Graesser et al. 1994) attempts to account for factors that predict inference generation during reading. The theory emphasizes the role of top-down, strategic processes in the construction of meaning, what has been termed Bsearch after meaning.^ Three assumptions define search after meaning. The first is the reader goal assumption, which suggests that readers construct meaning in accordance with their reading goals. The second is the coherence assumption, which suggests that readers construct meaning at both local and global levels. The third is the explanation assumption, which suggests that readers are driven to construct meaning that explains events they read. Even though the theory makes no concrete assumptions about EFs, it is reasonable to assume that shifting attention likely exerts an influence on the top-down, strategic processes that govern search after meaning.

Lastly, the Landscape Model (van den Broek et al. 1999) simulates the fluctuation of concept activation during reading. The Landscape Model is similar to the CI Model in that it assumes the same four sources of activation. The model also includes two important mechanisms, cohort activation and coherence-based retrieval. Cohort activation assumes that when a concept is activated, all other concepts that are also activated become associated with it (McClelland and Rumelhart 1985). Coherence-based retrieval assumes that the activation of text elements is in accordance with the readers' standards of coherence. In turn, standards of coherence refer to readers' implicit or explicit criteria for comprehension. Even though the Landscape Model makes no concrete assumptions about EFs, it is reasonable to assume that shifting likely exerts an influence on readers' standards of coherence, directing attention to information that aligns with readers' standards.



- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Thursday, April 26, 2018

Meta-analytic SEM of literacy and language development relations

Using Meta-analytic Structural Equation Modeling to Study Developmental Change in Relations Between Language and Literacy. Article link.

Jamie M. Quinn Richard K. Wagner

The purpose of this review was to introduce readers of Child Development to the meta-analytic structural equa-tion modeling (MASEM) technique. Provided are a background to the MASEM approach, a discussion of its utility in the study of child development, and an application of this technique in the study of reading compre-hension (RC) development. MASEM uses a two-stage approach: first, it provides a composite correlation matrix across included variables, and second, it fits hypothesized a priori models. The provided MASEM application used a large sample (N = 1,205,581) of students (ages 3.5–46.225) from 155 studies to investigate the factor structure and relations among components of RC. The practical implications of using this technique to study development are discussed.

Click on images to enlarge.









- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Tuesday, December 12, 2017

Research Byte: Reading and math achievement relations—a meta-analysis

The Association Between Arithmetic and Reading Performance in School: A Meta-Analytic Study

Article link

Vivian Singer and Kathernie Strasser

Many studies of school achievement find a significant association between reading and arithmetic achievement. The magnitude of the association varies widely across the studies, but the sources of this variation have not been identified. The purpose of this paper is to examine the magnitude and determinants of the relation between arithmetic and reading performance during elementary and middle school years. We meta-analyzed 210 correlations between math and reading measures, coming from 68 independent samples (the overall sample size was 58923 participants). The meta-analysis yielded an average correlation of 0.55 between math and reading measures. Among the moderators tested, only transparency of orthography and use of timed or untimed tests were significant in explaining the size of the correlation, with the largest correlations observed between timed measures of arithmetic and reading and between math and reading in opaque orthographies.

Implications for Instruction

The reported findings have practical implica-tions for the teaching of arithmetic and lan-guage. Specifically, they show that, although there may be some skills specific to language and to arithmetic performance, those specific factors can only partially account for the vari-ance in either of them. This highlights the im-portance of teaching general skills such as lan-guage processing and problem solving. Based on these findings, we consider it essential that, to make better educational decisions, educators as well as clinical practitioners take into account the wide arrange of possible factors that deter-mine performance in arithmetic and reading in school, instead of visualizing learning problems as the result of isolated deficits. In the curricu-lum, arithmetic and reading are presented as dissociated domains, but our results suggest that it may be beneficial to focus on their relation, be it from the perspective of common cognitive factors that influence both of them, or from a causal perspective where one of them influences the other. One corollary of the conception of arithmetic and reading as very separate domains may be the assumption that, because language is key for literacy development, it could not play a vital role for arithmetic learning, downplaying the importance of language for arithmetic learn-ing, and limiting the interventions available for teachers and special educators

Keywords: arithmetic, reading, schoolchildren

Click on image to enlarge



- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Monday, August 22, 2016

"Intelligent" intelligence testing with the WJ IV COG #7: Why do some individuals obtain markedly different scores on the various WJ IV Ga tests?

This is # 7 in the "Intelligent" intelligence testing with the WJ IV COG series at IQs Corner.  Copies of the PPT module can be downloaded by clicking on the LinkedIn icon in the right-hand corner of the slide show below  A PDF copy of all slides can be found here.

This module was developed in response to a thread on the IAPCHC listserv where an individual asked for help in understanding why the WJ IV Phonological Processing test score could be so much different (lower) that the WJ IV Sound Blending and Segmentation test scores.

Enjoy.



Tuesday, January 19, 2016

Research Byte: Study suggests writing places greater demands on working memory than reading and listening

Logo of advcogpsychAbout ACPSubscribeSumit a manuscriptACP Journal
 
Adv Cogn Psychol. 2015; 11(4): 147–155.
Published online 2015 Dec 31. doi:  10.5709/acp-0179-6
PMCID: PMC4710969

Writing, Reading, and Listening Differentially Overload Working Memory Performance Across the Serial Position Curve

Abstract

Previous research has assumed that writing is a cognitively complex task, but has not determined if writing overloads Working Memory more than reading and listening. To investigate this, participants completed three recall tasks. These were reading lists of words before recalling them, hearing lists of words before recalling them, and hearing lists of words and writing them as they heard them, then recalling them. The experiment involved serial recall of lists of 6 words. The hypothesis that fewer words would be recalled overall when writing was supported. Post-hoc analysis revealed the same pattern of results at individual serial positions (1 to 3). However, there was no difference between the three conditions at serial position 4, or between listening and writing at positions 5 and 6 which were both greater than recall in the reading condition. This suggests writing overloads working memory more than reading and listening, particularly in the early serial positions. The results show that writing interferes with working memory processes and so is not recommended when the goal is to immediately recall information.
Keywords: working memory, reading, listening, writing, serial recall

Sunday, January 10, 2016

Wednesday, September 23, 2015

Research Byte: Potential implications of performance on the WJ IV Oral Reading test in word reading and reading comprehension--indirect mediated effect of prosody


Click on image to enlarge

The WJ IV Achievement test battery includes a new reading test (Oral Reading) that contributes to the Reading Fluency cluster.  It is a measure of oral reading skill (story reading accuracy and prosody).  Briefly, prosody, in reading, refers to the expressiveness with which a student reads.  Why?  Because, fluent readers must be accurate readers.  The Oral Reading tests is a measure of fluency of oral expression in connected discourse. Fluent reading of the passages requires attention to the grammatical structure of prose. 
 
The recent study noted above may be relevant to the interpretation of the Oral Reading test.  This study suggests that prosody (prosodic sensitivity) does not have a direct causal effect on word reading or reading comprehension.  Rather, prosodic sensitivity may have an indirect impact on word reading via the mediating abilities of phonological and morphological awareness and an indirect impact on reading comprehension via word reading and listening comprehension.
 
This study suggests that the WJ IV phonological awareness tests (Phonetic Coding cluster; Oral Language tests of Segmentation and Sound Blending) should receive special attention when interpreting the impact of prosody (Oral Reading test) on the WJ IV Letter-Word Identification test (word reading).  In the case of performance on the WJ IV reading comprehension tests, the study suggests that special attention should be paid attention to prosody (Oral Reading) as mediated through a person's word reading (Letter-Word Identification) and listening comprehension (WJ IV Listening Comprehension cluster tests).

Conflict of interest disclosure.  I am a coauthor of the WJ IV.






Sunday, August 31, 2014

Sunday, August 24, 2014

Learning to read related to intellectual development - a longitudinal twins study

Wow...how about the complexity of that large path model?

Click on images to enlarge.












- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Wednesday, July 02, 2014

The story behind Dr. IQ McGrew's infamous WJ III and WJ IV test easel signature




I was pleased to receive my WJ IV test battery last week (I am a coauthor--conflict of interest disclosure). I was surprised to see that the publisher had lifted the author signatures that were on a limited edition printing of the WJ III back in 2001. So there I was looking at my barely third grade level signature....again. It made me laugh.

Today I received an email (the second) commenting on my signature. I provided the story. The person (semi-serious or tonque-n-check) was relieved as they thought it showed signs of a sub-clinical tremor and the possibility of signs of emerging problems.

The real truth is a funny story (in retrospect). When I was in grade school I struggled with "penmanship." I hated it. We used the Palmer Method. Everyday we had to practice making repetitive, rhythmic shapes.....and it was hell for me. My fingers turned red and I pressed so damn hard. Couple that with some spelling problems, and if I had been a child in today's schools, I would likely have been diagnosed with a mild spelling disability and dysgraphia. It likely is genetic, as my 37 year old son's handwriting (if you can call it that) is even worse and shows all the same signs.

Fast forward to the original request to supply the publisher with my signature for the WJ III. I spent the good part of a morning trying to make the best possible signature using different pens, pencils and multiple pieces of paper. I ended up with two choices. One would be my adult signature, which is a wild K followed by a few squiggly lines, with a very crude M somewhere in the middle. It is a way to disguise my horrible penmanship. I sent the publisher a page with various versions of the two types (doing it slow and doing it fast) and simply told them to pick one. They picked the one that was my best effort, and I swear it would not be judged as being beyond third grade level work.

So...now my signature is back to haunt me.

The good news is that in high school I completed a typing course. And damn, I was the fastest male student in my large class and earned many of those small words-per-minute pins. It was not the coolest thing to brag about in high school. Then, when PC's became available  later, all my problems were over (except for my spelling issues--which I think are more due to a very "busy mind"---and can be handled by spell check--if I bother to do it). I can type at a very fast rate on a computer keyboard. Technology saved me. I tell people that typing was the most important class I completed in high school. I have fond memories of Mrs. Carpenter, my HS typing teacher at Moorhead High. She made a difference.

So...before the rumors start the I am in some form of neurocognitive decline, I wanted to share "the rest of the story."


Thursday, June 19, 2014

Gc as higher order factor including other knowledge systems (verbal, Gq, Grw, Gkn, etc).

Interesting new study that provides some support for our (Schneider & McGrew, 2012 - largely the brilliance of Joel Schneider) conceptualization of Gc as a higher stratum factor, consistent with Cattell's provincial power gc, that includes language/verbal abilities, reading abilities (Grw), quantitative knowledge (Gq), domain-specific knowledge (Gkn), and possible other sources of knowledge. Our figure is at the end of this post. Click on images to enlarge.








Monday, November 18, 2013

Taub and Benson (2009). Gc and Gv related to rdg comp in college students

11-19-13 update.  I first posted this articleFYI without digesting the results in detail.  A subsequent comment (see comment section) brought my attention to a possible problem with a Heywood case.  I tend to agree with that comment upon further review.


A new study that shows direct effect of comprehension-knowledge (Gc) and visual-spatial processing (Gv), and indirect effect of general intelligence (g), on reading comprehension of college students. Click on images to enlarge



Sunday, July 22, 2012

Research byte: Support for validity of carefully constructed Cloze reading comprehension test.




Click on image to enlarge. The results are consistent with a white paper I wrote for the National Reading Panel over a decade ago


Posted using BlogPress from Kevin McGrew's iPad
www.themindhub.com

Monday, July 09, 2012

Research byte: Rise time perception and reading disabilities

Another article implicating auditory temporal processing abilities and readind disabilities...rise time perception problems.

Click image to enlarge



Posted using BlogPress from Kevin McGrew's iPad
www.themindhub.com

Sunday, July 01, 2012

Research bytes: Recent dyslexia research--Annals of Dyslexia




Daigle, D., Berthiaume, R., Plisson, A., & Demont, E. (2012). Graphophonological processes in dyslexic readers of French: A longitudinal study of the explicitness effect of tasks. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 82-99.

Deacon, S. H., Cook, K., & Parrila, R. (2012). Identifying high-functioning dyslexics: is self-report of early reading problems enough? Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 120-134.

Kast, M., Baschera, G. M., Gross, M., Jancke, L., & Meyer, M. (2012). Computer-based learning of spelling skills in children with and without dyslexia (Vol 61, pg 177, 2011). Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 135-136.

SuarezCoalla, P., & Cuetos, F. (2012). Reading strategies in Spanish developmental dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 71-81.

Wijnants, M. L., Hasselman, F., Cox, R. F. A., Bosman, A. M. T., & VanOrden, G. (2012). An interaction-dominant perspective on reading fluency and dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(2), 100-119.



Posted using BlogPress from Kevin McGrew's iPad
www.themindhub.com
Re

Friday, June 29, 2012

Research byte: Reading comprehension interventions--research synthesis




Double click on image to enlarge


Posted using BlogPress from Kevin McGrew's iPad
www.themindhub.com

Monday, January 23, 2012

Meta-analysis continues to confirm importance of phonemic awareness in reading...but...lets not overestimate the importance

Double click on image to enlarge the abstract.




Meta-analysis like this one are powerful studies that confirm the importance, in this case, for phonemic awareness (Ga-PC per CHC theory) for reading and dyslexia. However I believe that often the individual studies used as the bases of meta-analysis may overstate the importance of particular abilities due to the well-known problem of specification error in each studies design. I've blogged about this previously and won't take up bandwidth reiterating the importance of recognizing how specification error can cloud accurate interpretation of studies. The long and short of the issue is that manly reading and dyslexia studies during the past two decades have suffered from specification error....by only including indicators of the "hot and sexy" ability constructs in reading research and failing to include measures of known abilities that are also important in reading.

As a result of the "missing variable" problem, individual studies and, in this case, a meta-analysis of studies, most likely overstates the importance of the selected abilities analyzed...in this case phonemic awareness. For example, a recent synthesis by McGrew & Wendling (2010) demonstrated that other abilities often not included in the extant reading disability research (e.g., processing speed; associative memory; lexical knowledge) are important. If measures of these important reading-related abilities had been included in the studies summarized by this meta-analysis most likely the magnitude (effect sizes) of phonemic awareness would be lowered.

Yes...phonemic awareness (Ga-PC) is clearly important...but I caution readers to take the magnitude of the importance with a grain of salt as it is most likely somewhat less if all important reading-related variables had been included in the studies that are combined in this review



- Posted using BlogPress from Kevin McGrew's iPad