Tuesday, April 30, 2013

IQs Corner Recent Lit of Interest: 4-30-13

This weeks recent literature of interest is now available...with abstracts. Enjoy.

 

Sunday, April 28, 2013

Article Alert: Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading?

Why is rapid automatized naming related to reading?
Georgiou, George K.; Parrila, Rauno; Cui, Ying; Papadopoulos, Timothy C.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Vol. 115 Issue 1 – 2013: 218 - 225

10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.015

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0022096512002159

Saturday, April 27, 2013

Article Alert: WISC-IV Profiles Are Associated With Differences in Symptomatology and Outcome in Children With ADHD

WISC-IV Profiles Are Associated With Differences in Symptomatology and Outcome in Children With ADHD
Thaler, N. S.; Bello, D. T.; Etcoff, L. M.
Journal of Attention Disorders, Vol. 17 Issue 4 – 2013: 291 - 301

10.1177/1087054711428806

http://jad.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1177/1087054711428806

Article Alert: Sex differences on g and non-g intellectual performance reveal potential sources of STEM discrepancies

Sex differences on g and non-g intellectual performance reveal potential sources of STEM discrepancies
Lemos, Gina C.; Abad, Francisco J.; Almeida, Leandro S.; Colom, Roberto
Intelligence, Vol. 41 Issue 1 – 2013: 11 - 18

10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.009

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0160289612001225

Cycles in speed-working memory-G relations: Towards a developmental–differential theory of the mind

Cycles in speed-working memory-G relations: Towards a developmental–differential theory of the mind
Demetriou, Andreas; Spanoudis, George; Shayer, Michael; Mouyi, Antigoni; Kazi, Smaragda; Platsidou, Maria
Intelligence, Vol. 41 Issue 1 – 2013: 34 - 50

10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.010

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0160289612001237

Article Alert: The sources of the relationship between sustained attention and reasoning

The sources of the relationship between sustained attention and reasoning
Ren, Xuezhu; Schweizer, Karl; Xu, Fen
Intelligence, Vol. 41 Issue 1 – 2013: 51 - 58

10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.006

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0160289612001195

Article Alert: Sharing The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection via BrowZine

The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection
Jauk, Emanuel; Benedek, Mathias; Dunst, Beate; Neubauer, Aljoscha C.
Intelligence, Vol. 41 Issue 4 – 2013: 212 - 221

10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S016028961300024X

Article Alert: Investigating America's elite: Cognitive ability, education, and sex differences via BrowZine

Investigating America's elite: Cognitive ability, education, and sex differences
Wai, Jonathan
Intelligence, Vol. 41 Issue 4 – 2013: 203 - 211

10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.005

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0160289613000263

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.

Thursday, April 25, 2013

IQs Corner Recent Literature of Interest: 4-25-13




This weeks hand picked references, with abstracts, available here. Enjoy.


- Posted using BlogPress from my iPad

Wednesday, April 24, 2013

Article Alert: Spatial Mental Models: The Interaction of Presentation Format, Task Requirements and Availability of Working Memory Components via BrowZine

Spatial Mental Models: The Interaction of Presentation Format, Task Requirements and Availability of Working Memory Components
Picucci, Luciana; Gyselinck, Valérie; Piolino, Pascale; Nicolas, Serge; Bosco, Andrea
Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 27 Issue 3 – 2013: 314 - 327

10.1002/acp.2909

http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/acp.2909
*

Article Alert: Singing Numbers… in Cognitive Space - A Dual-Task Study of the Link Between Pitch, Space, and Numbers via BrowZine

Singing Numbers… in Cognitive Space - A Dual-Task Study of the Link Between Pitch, Space, and Numbers
Fischer, Martin H.; Riello, Marianna; Giordano, Bruno L.; Rusconi, Elena
Topics in Cognitive Science, Vol. 5 Issue 2 – 2013: 354 - 366

10.1111/tops.12017

http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/tops.12017

Article Alert: Seeking a Balance Between the Statistical and Scientific Elements in Psychometrics via BrowZine

Seeking a Balance Between the Statistical and Scientific Elements in Psychometrics
Wilson, Mark
Psychometrika, Vol. 78 Issue 2 – 2013: 211 - 236

10.1007/s11336-013-9327-3

http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s11336-013-9327-3

Article Alert: Brief Psychometric Analysis of the Self-Efficacy Parent Report Scale (SEPRS) via BrowZine

Brief Psychometric Analysis of the Self-Efficacy Parent Report Scale (SEPRS)
Erford, B. T.; Gavin, K.
Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, Vol. 46 Issue 2 – 2013: 79 - 87

10.1177/0748175612467464

http://mec.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1177/0748175612467464

Article Alert: Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity via BrowZine

Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity
von Bastian, Claudia C.; Oberauer, Klaus
Journal of Memory and Language, Vol. 69 Issue 1 – 2013: 36 - 58

10.1016/j.jml.2013.02.002

http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0749596X13000120

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.

Article Alert: Sharing Neuropsychological test performance of Spanish speakers: Is performance different across different Spanish-speaking subgroups? via BrowZine

Neuropsychological test performance of Spanish speakers: Is performance different across different Spanish-speaking subgroups?
Buré-Reyes, Annelly; Hidalgo-Ruzzante, Natalia; Vilar-López, Raquel; Gontier, Javier; Sánchez, Laura; Pérez-García, Miguel; Puente, Antonio E.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Vol. 35 Issue 4 – 2013: 404 - 412

10.1080/13803395.2013.778232

http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/13803395.2013.778232

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.
**

Article Alert: Sharing Multivariate Screening Model for Later Word Reading Achievement: Predictive Utility of Prereading Skills and Cognitive Ability via BrowZine

Multivariate Screening Model for Later Word Reading Achievement: Predictive Utility of Prereading Skills and Cognitive Ability
Macdonald, Heidi H.; Sullivan, Amanda L.; Watkins, Marley W.
Journal of Applied School Psychology, Vol. 29 Issue 1 – 2013: 52 - 71

10.1080/15377903.2013.751476

http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/15377903.2013.751476

Accessed with BrowZine, supported by University of Minnesota.


*****************************************
Kevin McGrew, Phd.
Educational Psychologist
Institute for Applied Psychometrics
Director IAP
www.themindhub.com
*****************************************

Article Alert: How Much Power and Speed Is Measured in This Test?, Partchev, I.; De Boeck, P.; Steyer, R.

http://asm.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1177/1073191111411658


*****************************************
Kevin McGrew, Phd.
Educational Psychologist
Institute for Applied Psychometrics
Director IAP
www.themindhub.com
*****************************************

Thank you IQs Corner readers - 1/2 million and counting


Sometime during the past week IQ's Corner hit the 1/2 million mark on number of visitors (see report I received this morning).  And, the "hits" are inching closer to the million mark. I want to thank all my readers and visitors.  These numbers keep me blogging.

Subject: GoStats: weekly stats

Site: "IQs Corner" 

Total counter state:

   Hits:         890809
   Hosts:        388172
   Visitors:     500271
   Sessions:     571694





Sunday, April 21, 2013

Four new Flynn effect articles

These new articles will be added to the Flynn Effect Archive Project soon.

(Click on images to enlarge).

 

Saturday, April 20, 2013

Article: Neuronal Connections and the Mind - The Connectome | Jon Lieff, M.D.

Excellent overview of the Human Connectome project 
 
Neuronal Connections and the Mind - The Connectome | Jon Lieff, M.D.
http://jonlieffmd.com/blog/neuronal-connections-and-the-mind-the-connectome

Sent via Flipboard



Article: Training the brain to improve on new tasks

This study is getting all kinds of social media attention. I am particularly keen on the discussion of increased parietal and frontal connectivity as that is a major cornerstone of a recent MindHub Pub 2 (available at www.themindhub.com:  http://themindhub.com/research-reports ).

That model focuses on a three level explanation of the IM effect which can be viewed as a general explanation of working memory training in general.  This research provides additional support for the P-FIT model of intelligence.




Article: Beyond IQ: What Makes The Difference Between Success And Failure In Life





*****************************************
Kevin McGrew, Phd.
Educational Psychologist
Institute for Applied Psychometrics
Director IAP
*****************************************

Journal Alert - INTELLIGENCE

> Title:
> Faster on easy items, more accurate on difficult ones: Cognitive ability and performance on a task of varying difficulty
>
> Authors:
> Dodonova, YA; Dodonov, YS
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):1-10; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Using more complex items than those commonly employed within the
> information-processing approach, but still easier than those used in
> intelligence tests, this study analyzed how the association between
> processing speed and accuracy level changes as the difficulty of the
> items increases. The study involved measuring cognitive ability using
> Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices and examining changes in the
> accuracy-ability and speed-ability correlations due to the increasing
> difficulty of the items. As expected, high-ability individuals
> demonstrated a higher accuracy rate and faster performance than
> low-ability individuals. The accuracy-ability association became
> stronger as the item difficulty increased. The speed-ability
> correlations tended to decrease in absolute values as the item
> difficulty increased, although this trend, when formally examined, was
> non-significant.
> For a more precise analysis, the entire variance of each response time
> and accuracy was decomposed into the components that described constancy
> in performance over difficulty levels and the rate of change in
> performance caused by increasing task difficulty. Estimating these
> components for both response latencies and accuracy scores within the
> same latent growth model demonstrated that the speed-accuracy
> correlation was evident only for baseline performance; the rate of
> slowing with increasing task difficulty was not correlated with the
> respective rate of change in accuracy level. High-ability participants
> differed from low-ability participants in the speed (not accuracy) of
> baseline performance and in the rate of difficulty-related change in
> accuracy (not in processing speed). (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights
> reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 11-18 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700002
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Sex differences on g and non-g intellectual performance reveal potential sources of STEM discrepancies
>
> Authors:
> Lemos, GC; Abad, FJ; Almeida, LS; Colom, R
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):11-18; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> The analysis of sex differences in cognitive abilities is largely
> confusing because these differences are masked by the pervasive
> influence of the general factor of intelligence (g). In this study a
> battery of five reasoning tests (abstract [AR], numerical [NR], verbal
> [VR], mechanical [MR], and spatial [SR]) was completed by a sample of
> 3233 young and old adolescents representative of the population. Using a
> latent variable approach, mean differences on the general factor were
> estimated after examining measurement invariance. Results show that the
> difference, favoring boys in latent g increases with age from two to
> four IQ points. Further, boys outperform girls in all the subtests and
> the observed differences were generally explained by g. However,
> mechanical reasoning is a systematic and strong exception to this
> finding. For the young adolescents, the observed difference in MR is
> equivalent to 10 IQ points, and this difference increases to 13 IQ
> points for the old adolescents. Only 1 (young) or 2 (old) IQ points of
> the sex difference in MR can be accounted for by g. The findings suggest
> that the persistent - and usually neglected average large advantage of
> boys in mechanical reasoning (MR) - orthogonal to g - might be behind
> their higher presence in STEM (science, technology, engineering, and
> math) disciplines. A new look at this relevant social issue is proposed
> in this study. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 19-24 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700003
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Secular trends in cognitive test performance: Swedish conscript data 1970-1993
>
> Authors:
> Ronnlund, M; Carlstedt, B; Blomstedt, Y; Nilsson, LG; Weinehall, L
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):19-24; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> We investigated time-related patterns in levels of cognitive performance
> during the period from 1970 to 1993 based on data from Swedish draft
> boards. The conscripts, including more than a million 18-19-year old
> men, had taken one of two versions of the Swedish enlistment battery
> (SEB67; 1970-1979 or SEB80; 1980-1993), each composed of four subtests.
> The results revealed significant Flynn effects, with estimated gains of
> 1.2-1.5 IQ-units per decade. The effect seem to hold across ability
> levels, even though tendencies of more pronounced effects in the lower
> half of the ability distribution was observed. The largest gains were
> for visuospatial tests (Paper Form Board and Metal Folding), with little
> change, even slight losses during the second sub-period, for tests of
> verbal knowledge (Concept Discrimination and Synonyms) and a mixed
> pattern for a test of technical comprehension (losses followed by
> gains). Finally, comparisons of trends in cognitive performance and in
> standing height show that the gains in cognitive performance over the
> years from 1980 to 1993 occurred in the absence of overall gains in
> height which speaks against nutrition as the cause of the Flynn effects.
> (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 25-33 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700004
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Problems in deriving Italian regional differences in intelligence from 2009 PISA data
>
> Authors:
> Cornoldi, C; Giofre, D; Martini, A
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):25-33; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Recent results of international assessment programs (e.g., PISA) have
> shown a large difference in high school students' performance between
> northern and southern Italy. On this basis, it has been argued that the
> discrepancy reflects differences in average intelligence of the
> inhabitants of regions and is associated with genetic factors (Lynn,
> 2010a, 2012). This paper provides evidence in contrast to this
> conclusion by arguing that the use of PISA data to make inferences about
> regional differences in intelligence is questionable, and in any case,
> both PISA and other recent surveys on achievement of North and South
> Italy students offer some results that do not support Lynn's
> conclusions. In particular, a 2006-2009 PISA data comparison shows a
> relevant decrease in the North-South difference in only three years,
> particularly evident in the case of a single region (Apulia). Other
> large surveys (including INVALSI-2011) offer different results; age
> differences suggest that schooling could have an important role. (C)
> 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 34-50 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700005
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Cycles in speed-working memory-G relations: Towards a developmental-differential theory of the mind
>
> Authors:
> Demetriou, A; Spanoudis, G; Shayer, M; Mouyi, A; Kazi, S; Platsidou, M
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):34-50; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> This article presents three studies, two of them longitudinal, which
> investigated the relations between age, processing speed, working memory
> (WM), and fluid intelligence (g(f)) from 4 to 16 years of age.
> Structural equation modeling showed that speed was a powerful covariate
> of age (similar to-.6 to -.7) from 4 to 13 years, declining thereafter
> (to similar to-.2). WM was stably related to speed through the whole
> age-span studied (similar to-.4 to -.5). A large part (59%) of
> age-related changes in gf (83%) from 4 to 7 years and a lower but
> significant part later on, especially in adolescence (similar to 10-20%
> out of similar to 40-50%), were mediated by WM. However, with speed and
> age controlled, WM was almost fully commensurate with g(f)(similar
> to-.9), from about the age of 8-9 years onwards. A series of models
> suggested an ever present efficiency level defined by speed and control
> and a representational level defined by WM and g(f), which are
> increasingly differentiated with development. All processes develop in
> cycles concerted by a dynamic G. Change in each process over time
> originated from within the processes themselves and G, in proportions
> varying with developmental phase. Overall, speed signified
> age-associated changes in processing capabilities, partly expressed in
> WM expansions and g(f) reconstructions. An overarching model is proposed
> integrating differential with developmental theories of human
> intelligence. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 51-58 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700006
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> The sources of the relationship between sustained attention and reasoning
>
> Authors:
> Ren, XZ; Schweizer, K; Xu, F
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):51-58; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Although a substantial relationship of sustained attention and reasoning
> was consistently found, little is known about what drives this
> relationship. The present study aims at revealing the underlying sources
> that are responsible for the relationship by means of an integrative
> approach combining experimental manipulation and psychometric modeling.
> A sample of 4551 participants falling into three age groups (aged 12,
> 13, and 14 years) completed a sustained attention test and three
> reasoning subtests. The attention test comprised four treatment levels
> designed by manipulating the number of distracters. Confirmatory factor
> models were applied to decompose the variance of the attention data into
> three components: executive control due to stimulus manipulation,
> automatic learning as the result of extended practice, and a general
> component independent of any changes. The results showed that in all age
> groups the component of execution control showed the strongest link with
> reasoning although the learning component also significantly related
> with reasoning. In contrast, a non-significant link was observed between
> the general component and reasoning. These results indicated that the
> sources of the relationship between sustained attention and reasoning
> are mainly due to executive control of attention, and additionally to a
> learning process. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 59-66 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700007
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Average county-level IQ predicts county-level disadvantage and several county-level mortality risk rates
>
> Authors:
> Barnes, JC; Beaver, KM; Boutwell, BB
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):59-66; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Research utilizing individual-level data has reported a link between
> intelligence (IQ) scores and health problems, including early mortality
> risk. A growing body of evidence has found similar associations at
> higher levels of aggregation such as the state- and national-level. At
> the same time, individual-level research has suggested the IQ-mortality
> risk association may be mediated by socioeconomic status, but no
> aggregate research has considered this possibility. This paper extended
> the current knowledge base in two important ways: 1) by analyzing the
> association between county-level IQ and county-level mortality risk; and
> 2) by testing a theoretical model where county IQ influences county
> disadvantage which, in turn, influences county mortality risk. The
> findings indicated a consistent relationship between county IQ and
> several measures of county mortality risk. The IQ-mortality risk
> association was mediated by county disadvantage for some county
> mortality risk measures but not others, suggesting the relationship
> between county IQ and county mortality risk is more nuanced than was
> hypothesized. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 67-69 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700008
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Dysgenic fertility, intelligence and family size in Libya
>
> Authors:
> Al-Shahomee, AA; Lynn, R; Abdalla, SEG
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):67-69; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> The Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) was administered to a sample of
> 592 16 year old school students in Libya. There was a small negative
> correlation of -0.14 between SPM scores and the number of siblings,
> indicating only marginal dysgenic fertility. Supplementary material
> giving the data is given online. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights
> reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 70-84 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700009
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Cognitive sex differences in reasoning tasks: Evidence from Brazilian samples of educational settings
>
> Authors:
> Flores-Mendoza, C; Widaman, KF; Rindermann, H; Primi, R; Mansur-Alves,
> M; Pena, CC
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):70-84; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Sex differences on the Attention Test (AC), the Raven's Standard
> Progressive Matrices (SPM), and the Brazilian Cognitive Battery (BPR5),
> were investigated using four large samples (total N = 6780), residing in
> the states of Minas Gerais and Sao Paulo. The majority of samples used,
> which were obtained from educational settings, could be considered a
> nonprobability sampling. Females outperformed males on the AC (by 2 IQ
> points), whereas males slightly outperformed females on the SPM (by 1.5
> IQ points). On the BPR5, sex differences favoring males were
> statistically significant (on average 6.2 IQ points). The largest
> difference was in Mechanical Reasoning (13 IQ points), and the smallest
> was in Spatial Reasoning (5 IQ points). In addition, two methods were
> adopted for determining whether sex differences existed at the level of
> general intelligence. First, a g factor score was estimated after
> principal axis factoring of test scores. Men had an advantage of 3.8 IQ
> points (statistically significant) on the g score, which was reduced to
> 2.7 IQ points (not significant), when the g score was estimated without
> including Mechanical Reasoning. Second, a confirmatory factor analysis
> approach was conducted that allowed testing of mean differences at the
> latent variable level. Again, sex differences favoring males were found
> (0.23 or 3.44 IQ points). Regarding educational and SES variables, some
> sex differences favoring males were found in the SPM and in the BPR5. In
> general, our results agree with studies that identify small, but
> consistent cognitive sex differences in reasoning tasks. Societal
> implications are discussed. (C) 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 85-86 (Book Review)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700010
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Review of: Intelligence. A unifying construct for the social sciences
>
> Authors:
> Gale, CR
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):85-86; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 87-87 (Book Review)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700011
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Good Blood, Bad Blood: Science, Nature, and the Myth of the Kallikaks
>
> Authors:
> Johnson, W
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):87-87; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 88-89 (Biographical-Item)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316512700012
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> John Philippe Rushton, 1943-2012 Obituary
>
> Authors:
> Lynn, R
>
> Source:
> *INTELLIGENCE*, 41 (1):88-89; JAN-FEB 2013
>
> ========================================================================
> *Order Full Text*
> All Customers
> --------------
> Please contact your library administrator, or person(s) responsible for
> document delivery, to find out more about your organization.s policy for
> obtaining the full text of the above articles. If your organization does not
> have a current document delivery provider, you can order the document from our
> document delivery service TS Doc. To order a copy of the article(s) you wish
> to receive, please go to www.contentscm.com and enter the citation information for
> each document. A price quote for each item will be given and you will need a
> credit card to complete your order request.
>
> TS Doc Customers
> --------------
> TS Doc customers can purchase the full text of an article using their
> TS Doc account. Go to www.contentscm.com and login using your TS Doc logon ID
> and password. Copy & paste the citation into the parser (Order by Citation)
> or enter the citation information above on the web order form (Order by Form.)
> A quote will be given for each item and your company will be invoiced as
> specified in your TS Doc agreement.
>
>
> If you would like to supply contact information for TS Doc, here is the updated info:
> Product name: TS Doc
> Customer Service: customerservice@infotrieve.com or (800) 603-4367
>
>
> ========================================================================
> *Support Contact Information*
> If you have any questions, please open a support ticket at http://ip-science.thomsonreuters.com/techsupport/.
> Telephone numbers for your local support team are also available here.
> ========================================================================

Current Directions in Psychological Science Table of Contents for Special Issue on the Teenage Brain: 1 April 2013; Vol. 22, No. 2


Current Directions in Psychological Science Online Table of Contents Alert

Current Directions in Psychological Science Online Table of Contents Alert

A new issue of Current Directions in Psychological Science is available online:
Special Issue on the Teenage Brain:
April 2013; Vol. 22, No. 2


The below Table of Contents is available online at: http://cdp.sagepub.com/content/vol22/issue2/?etoc


Articles
Introduction to Special Issue on the Teenage Brain
Randall W. Engle
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 79
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/full/22/2/79

The Teenage Brain: An Overview
B. J. Casey
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 80-81
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/full/22/2/80

The Teenage Brain: Self Control
B. J. Casey and Kristina Caudle
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 82-87
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/82

The Teenage Brain: Sensitivity to Rewards
Adriana Galván
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 88-93
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/88

The Teenage Brain: Cognitive Control and Motivation
Beatriz Luna, David J. Paulsen, Aarthi Padmanabhan, and Charles Geier
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 94-100
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/94

The Teenage Brain: Functional Connectivity
Nico U. F. Dosenbach, Steven E. Petersen, and Bradley L. Schlaggar
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 101-107
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/101

The Teenage Brain: A Neuroeconomic Approach to Adolescent Decision Making
Anna C. K. Van Duijvenvoorde and Eveline A. Crone
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 108-113
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/108

The Teenage Brain: Peer Influences on Adolescent Decision Making
Dustin Albert, Jason Chein, and Laurence Steinberg
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 114-120
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/114

The Teenage Brain: Sensitivity to Social Evaluation
Leah H. Somerville
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 121-127
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/121

The Teenage Brain: Social Reorientation and the Adolescent Brain—The Role of Gonadal Hormones in the Male Syrian Hamster
Kayla De Lorme, Margaret R. Bell, and Cheryl L. Sisk
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 128-133
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/128

The Teenage Brain: Surging Hormones—Brain-Behavior Interactions During Puberty
Jiska S. Peper and Ronald E. Dahl
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 134-139
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/134

The Teenage Brain: The Stress Response and the Adolescent Brain
Russell D. Romeo
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 140-145
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/140

The Teenage Brain: Altered Fear in Humans and Mice
Siobhan S. Pattwell, B. J. Casey, and Francis S. Lee
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 146-151
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/146

The Teenage Brain: Adolescents and Alcohol
Linda Spear
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 152-157
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/152

The Teenage Brain: Adolescent Brain Research and the Law
Richard J. Bonnie and Elizabeth S. Scott
Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013;22 158-161
http://cdp.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/22/2/158


HOME | ONLINEFIRST | ALL ISSUES | SUBSCRIBE | rss RSS | EMAIL ALERTS | FEEDBACK

Tuesday, April 16, 2013

Article: Charts: How parents around the world describe their children


Charts: How parents around the world describe their children
http://qz.com/74136/charts-how-parents-around-the-world-describe-their-children/

Sent via Flipboard




*****************************************
Kevin McGrew, Phd.
Educational Psychologist
Institute for Applied Psychometrics
Director IAP
*****************************************

Monday, April 15, 2013

In Defense of Working Memory Training | Beautiful Minds, Scientific American Blog Network

Excellent more balanced SA-MIND article on brain training programs. Notice the positive mention of Interactive Metronome..with a link to my recent MindHub Pub Report (conflict of interest statement: I am a paid external consultant to IM. More info can be found at www.mindhub.com)


http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/beautiful-minds/2013/04/15/in-defense-of-working-memory-training/


Thursday, April 11, 2013

Journal Alert - NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA

> Title:
> Metacognitive and online error awareness deficits after prefrontal cortex lesions
>
> Authors:
> Hoerold, D; Pender, NP; Robertson, IH
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):385-391; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Awareness of deficits after brain injury represents a significant
> clinical and theoretical challenge, but relatively little is known about
> the neuroanatomical correlates of specific types of deficit awareness.
> We examined the awareness correlates of left versus right prefrontal
> cortex lesions in comparison to left and right posterior lesions
> including two types of awareness measures-metacognitive and online error
> monitoring. Frontal lobe frontal lesion patients exhibited impaired
> metacognitive awareness and also showed deficits in monitoring errors as
> they occurred. In addition, frontal lobe lesion patients also showed
> reduced autonomic response to aware errors. Online and metacognitive
> awareness were not, however, significantly correlated, suggesting that
> distinct neuroanatomical systems may underpin these two types of
> awareness deficit. We hypothesize that while metacognitive awareness
> depends on both left and right frontal regions, accurate
> moment-to-moment processing of errors depends more on the right than on
> the left prefrontal cortex. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 392-399 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000002
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Using a startling acoustic stimulus to investigate underlying mechanisms of bradykinesia in Parkinson's disease
>
> Authors:
> Carlsen, AN; Almeida, QJ; Franks, IM
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):392-399; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Delays in the initiation of a movement response and slowness during
> movement are among the hallmark motor symptoms in patients with
> Parkinson's disease (PD). These impairments may result from deficits in
> neural structures related to perception, response programming, response
> initiation, or a combination of all three. However, the relative impact
> of each process on movement control in PD is still unclear. The present
> study investigated which processes might be responsible for the observed
> slowness. Patients performed a simple reaction time (RT) task involving
> arm extension where the normal 82 dB acoustic "go" signal was
> unexpectedly replaced with a 124 dB startling acoustic stimulus (SAS) on
> selected trials. The SAS was used as a probe of motor preparatory state
> since it has been shown to act as a subcortically-mediated involuntarily
> trigger for actions that are sufficiently prepared and waiting to be
> initiated by normal cortical processes. It was expected that release of
> the voluntary response by startle would not occur in PD patients if
> bradykinetic symptoms were attributable primarily to motor programming
> deficits. In contrast, results clearly showed that when a SAS was
> presented, the prepared response was elicited at a significantly shorter
> latency. In addition, the amplitude and timing pattern of EMG output
> appeared to be improved compared to control, resulting in a faster, more
> normalized movement. These results suggest that in PD patients motor
> programming processes are relatively intact, while the dysfunctional
> basal ganglia likely assert an inhibitory effect on the thalamo-cortical
> connections responsible for the initiation of motor acts. (c) 2012
> Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 400-409 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000003
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Reversal of the face-inversion effect in N170 under unconscious visual processing
>
> Authors:
> Suzuki, M; Noguchi, Y
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):400-409; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Many studies using electroencephalography consistently reported a larger
> N170 (N1) response in the visual cortices to inverted than upright face
> images (the face inversion effect in N1, FIE-N1). Here we report this
> robust effect is diminished and even reversed when face stimuli are
> processed unconsciously. We measured visual-evoked potentials to neutral
> faces either visible or rendered invisible by an inter-ocular
> suppression. In visible condition, we observed a larger N1 to inverted
> than upright faces, which replicated the traditional FIE-N1. When those
> faces became invisible, however, neural responses to the inverted faces
> were greatly reduced compared to visible condition, whereas those to the
> invisible upright faces were relatively preserved. Consequently, N1
> amplitudes were found to be larger in upright, rather than inverted,
> faces in invisible condition, which was opposite to the traditional
> FIE-N1 (upright < inverted) in visible condition. Those results
> highlighted a special mechanism in the brain for the processing of the
> upright, but not inverted, face (e.g. fusiform face area) that retains
> vigorous responses even when the face becomes invisible. (c) 2012
> Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 410-417 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000004
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Human parietal and primary motor cortical interactions are selectively modulated during the transport and grip formation of goal-directed hand actions
>
> Authors:
> Vesia, M; Bolton, DA; Mochizuki, G; Staines, WR
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):410-417; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Posterior parietal cortex (PPC) constitutes a critical cortical node in
> the sensorimotor system in which goal-directed actions are computed.
> This information then must be transferred into commands suitable for
> hand movements to the primary motor cortex (M1). Complexity arises
> because reach-to-grasp actions not only require directing the hand
> towards the object (transport component), but also preshaping the hand
> according to the features of the object (grip component). Yet, the
> functional influence that specific PPC regions exert over ipsilateral M1
> during the planning of different hand movements remains unclear in
> humans. Here we manipulated transport and grip components of
> goal-directed hand movements and exploited paired-pulse transcranial
> magnetic stimulation (ppTMS) to probe the functional interactions
> between M1 and two different PPC regions, namely superior
> parieto-occipital cortex (SPOC) and the anterior region of the
> intraparietal sulcus (aIPS), in the left hemisphere. We show that when
> the extension of the arm is required to contact a target object, SPOC
> selectively facilitates motor evoked potentials, suggesting that SPOC-M1
> interactions are functionally specific to arm transport. In contrast, a
> different pathway, linking the aIPS and ipsilateral M1, shows enhanced
> functional connections during the sensorimotor planning of grip. These
> results support recent human neuroimaging findings arguing for
> specialized human parietal regions for the planning of arm transport and
> hand grip during goal-directed actions. Importantly, they provide new
> insight into the causal influences these different parietal regions
> exert over ipsilateral motor cortex for specific types of planned hand
> movements. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 418-424 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000005
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Hypofrontal activity during word retrieval in older adults: A near-infrared spectroscopy study
>
> Authors:
> Obayashi, S; Hara, Y
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):418-424; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> The supplementary motor area (SMA) has been regarded as a third speech
> area. The SMA is anatomically classified into two regions, pre-SMA and
> SMA proper, but the functional specialization of speech production
> between the two regions remains unknown. Although word retrieval
> difficulties were often observed in older adults, there was no report as
> to whether the SMA would be involved in the retrieval difficulties. We
> focused on the SMA as a function of word production and then used
> near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) with the verbal fluency task (VFT) to
> explore the possible mechanism underlying the retrieval difficulties
> related to aging. Based on the anatomical differences within the SMA, we
> relied on region-of-interest (ROI) analysis to compare the brain
> activation patterns in the SMA during VFT between 11 healthy elder and
> 11 younger subjects in the situation where both groups show comparable
> task performance. Notably, the anterior VFT-related SMA response was
> more robust in the younger than in the elder group. Furthermore,
> anterior SMA responses in the elder group may only have a positive
> correlation with the VFT performance. The findings imply that anterior
> SMA hypoactivity in elders may cause word retrieval difficulties, while
> bilateral prefrontal cortices, having close connection with the pre-SMA,
> may contribute to the compensatory process that enables equivalent
> performance of the elder group with the younger one. (c) 2012 Elsevier
> Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 425-436 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000006
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Reading aloud: A psychophysiological investigation in children
>
> Authors:
> Chiarenza, GA; Olgiati, P; Trevisan, C; De Marchi, I; Casarotto, S
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):425-436; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> This study investigated the electrophysiological responses to
> single-letter reading in children (reading-related potentials) and
> explored the morphological differences between covert and overt reading
> conditions.
> Sixty-five healthy children (6-13 years) participated in this study.
> Reading-related potentials were recorded during visual stimulation with
> single Italian alphabetic letters. Stimuli were displayed for 5 ms
> either automatically at a randomly uttered time lag or upon voluntary
> self-paced button press by children. In the covert conditions, children
> had to passively look at single letters, while in the overt conditions
> children were required to read aloud the letters. Electromyographic
> activity of the forearm and lips was additionally recorded during all
> tasks.
> Superimposition of reading-related potentials with the electromyographic
> activity of forearm and lips during self-paced reading aloud allowed to
> segregate the reading-related components into four periods: preparatory,
> pre-lexical, lexical and post-lexical. Reading-related potentials of the
> preparatory period can be related to preparation/intention to read,
> those of the pre-lexical period to visual-perceptual processes, those of
> the lexical period to the external/internal reafferent activity and
> those of the post-lexical period to the feedback processes following
> task completion.
> Analysis of variance showed a significant interaction of reading-related
> components with electrode locations and task conditions in all periods.
> The systematic characterization of the neurophysiological correlates of
> the elementary association between letters and sounds is helpful to
> highlight the neurobiological and functional basis of reading in healthy
> as well as impaired readers, for possibly developing
> neurophysiologically grounded rehabilitation therapies and further
> improving the explanatory models of dyslexia. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All
> rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 437-447 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000007
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> The contents of predictions in sentence comprehension: Activation of the shape of objects before they are referred to
>
> Authors:
> Rommers, J; Meyer, AS; Praamstra, P; Huettig, F
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):437-447; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> When comprehending concrete words, listeners and readers can activate
> specific visual information such as the shape of the words' referents.
> In two experiments we examined whether such information can be activated
> in an anticipatory fashion. In Experiment 1, listeners' eye movements
> were tracked while they were listening to sentences that were predictive
> of a specific critical word (e.g., "moon" in "In 1969 Neil Armstrong was
> the first man to set foot on the moon"). 500 ms before the acoustic
> onset of the critical word, participants were shown four-object displays
> featuring three unrelated distractor objects and a critical object,
> which was either the target object (e.g., moon), an object with a
> similar shape (e.g., tomato), or an unrelated control object (e.g.,
> rice). In a time window before shape information from the spoken target
> word could be retrieved, participants already tended to fixate both the
> target and the shape competitors more often than they fixated the
> control objects, indicating that they had anticipatorily activated the
> shape of the upcoming word's referent. This was confirmed in Experiment
> 2, which was an ERP experiment without picture displays. Participants
> listened to the same lead-in sentences as in Experiment 1. The
> sentence-final words corresponded to the predictable target, the shape
> competitor, or the unrelated control object (yielding, for instance, "In
> 1969 Neil Armstrong was the first man to set foot on the
> moon/tomato/rice"). N400 amplitude in response to the final words was
> significantly attenuated in the shape-related compared to the unrelated
> condition. Taken together, these results suggest that listeners can
> activate perceptual attributes of objects before they are referred to in
> an utterance. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 448-456 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000008
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> An fMRI study of episodic encoding across the lifespan: Changes in subsequent memory effects are evident by middle-age
>
> Authors:
> Park, H; Kennedy, KM; Rodrigue, KM; Hebrank, A; Park, DC
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):448-456; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Although it is well-documented that there are age differences between
> young and older adults in neural activity associated with successful
> memory formation (positive subsequent memory effects), little is known
> about how this activation differs across the lifespan, as few studies
> have included middle-aged adults. The present study investigated the
> effect of age on neural activity during episodic encoding using a
> cross-sectional lifespan sample (20-79 years old, N = 192) from the
> Dallas Lifespan Brain Study. We report four major findings. First, in a
> contrast of remembered vs. forgotten items, a decrease in neural
> activity occurred with age in bilateral occipito-temporo-parietal
> regions. Second, when we contrasted forgotten with remembered items
> (negative subsequent memory), the primary difference was found between
> middle and older ages. Third, there was evidence for age equivalence in
> hippocampal regions, congruent with previous studies. Finally,
> low-memory-performers showed negative subsequent memory differences by
> middle age, whereas high memory performers did not demonstrate these
> differences until older age. Taken together, these findings delineate
> the importance of a lifespan approach to understanding neurocognitive
> aging and, in particular, the importance of a middle-age sample in
> revealing different trajectories. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights
> reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 457-463 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000009
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Neuroanatomical correlates of biological motion detection
>
> Authors:
> Gilaie-Dotan, S; Kanai, R; Bahrami, B; Rees, G; Saygin, AP
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):457-463; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Biological motion detection is both commonplace and important, but there
> is great inter-individual variability in this ability, the neural basis
> of which is currently unknown. Here we examined whether the behavioral
> variability in biological motion detection is reflected in brain
> anatomy. Perceptual thresholds for detection of biological motion and
> control conditions (non-biological object motion detection and motion
> coherence) were determined in a group of healthy human adults (n = 31)
> together with structural magnetic resonance images of the brain. Voxel
> based morphometry analyzes revealed that gray matter volumes of left
> posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and left ventral premotor
> cortex (vPMC) significantly predicted individual differences in
> biological motion detection, but showed no significant relationship with
> performance on the control tasks. Our study reveals a neural basis
> associated with the inter-individual variability in biological motion
> detection, reliably linking the neuroanatomical structure of left pSTS
> and vPMC with biological motion detection performance. (c) 2012 Elsevier
> Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 464-471 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000010
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Transitive inference reasoning is impaired by focal lesions in parietal cortex rather than rostrolateral prefrontal cortex
>
> Authors:
> Waechter, RL; Goel, V; Raymont, V; Kruger, F; Grafman, J
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):464-471; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Transitive inference reasoning involves the examination and comparison
> of a given number of relational pairs in order to understand overall
> group hierarchy (e.g., A > B, B > C, C > D; therefore is A > D?). A
> number of imaging studies have demonstrated the role of the parietal
> cortex for resolving transitive inferences. Some studies also identify
> the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex as being critical for "relational
> integration" processes supporting transitive reasoning. To clarify this
> issue, we carried out a transitive inference study involving
> neurological patients with focal lesions to the rostrolateral prefrontal
> (n = 5) or parietal cortices (n = 7), as well as normal controls (n =
> 6). The patients and controls were statistically matched on age,
> education, pre-injury IQ general memory, working memory, and
> performance/full IQ, though the rostrolateral patients did score
> significantly higher than the normal controls on verbal IQ. Results
> indicate that patients with focal lesions to the parietal cortex were
> impaired in the task relative to both the patients with focal lesions to
> rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and the control group, and there was no
> difference in task performance between the rostrolateral prefrontal and
> the control groups. This result continued to hold after controlling for
> verbal IQ as a covariate. These findings point to a critical role for
> the parietal cortex, rather than the rostrolateral prefrontal, in
> transitive inference. Since the groups performed similarly on a working
> memory task, working memory cannot fully account for the result,
> suggesting a specific role of parietal cortex in transitive inference.
> (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 472-481 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000011
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Probing the neurocognitive trajectories of children's reading skills
>
> Authors:
> Talcott, JB; Witton, C; Stein, JF
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):472-481; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Emerging evidence of the high variability in the cognitive skills and
> deficits associated with reading achievement and dysfunction promotes
> both a more dimensional view of the risk factors involved, and the
> importance of discriminating between trajectories of impairment. Here we
> examined reading and component orthographic and phonological skills
> alongside measures of cognitive ability and auditory and visual sensory
> processing in a large group of primary school children between the ages
> of 7 and 12 years. We identified clusters of children with pseudoword or
> exception word reading scores at the 10th percentile or below relative
> to their age group, and a group with poor skills on both tasks. Compared
> to age-matched and reading-level controls, groups of children with more
> impaired exception word reading were best described by a trajectory of
> developmental delay, whereas readers with more impaired pseudoword
> reading or combined deficits corresponded more with a pattern of
> atypical development. Sensory processing deficits clustered within both
> of the groups with putative atypical development: auditory
> discrimination deficits with poor phonological awareness skills;
> impairments of visual motion processing in readers with broader and more
> severe patterns of reading and cognitive impairments. Sensory deficits
> have been variably associated with developmental impairments of literacy
> and language; these results suggest that such deficits are also likely
> to cluster in children with particular patterns of reading difficulty.
> (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 482-487 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000012
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Conscious processing during retrieval can occur in early and late visual regions
>
> Authors:
> Thakral, PP; Slotnick, SD; Schacter, DL
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):482-487; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Previous evidence has suggested a functional-anatomic dissociation
> between conscious and nonconscious processing during retrieval where
> early visual regions BA17/18 are associated with nonconscious processing
> and late visual regions BA19/37 are associated with conscious
> processing. However, evidence for this dissociation has only been
> observed using a limited number of experimental paradigms. In the
> present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, we tested
> the hypothesis that conscious processing during retrieval can occur in
> BA17/18 using memorial paradigms that recruited processing in these
> early visual regions. During the encoding phase of Experiment 1,
> abstract shapes with colored and oriented internal lines were presented
> to the left and right of fixation. During the retrieval phase, old
> shapes and new shapes were presented at fixation and participants
> classified each item as "old-left", "old-right", or "new". The contrast
> of spatial memory-hits > spatial memory-misses (with accurate item
> memory) produced activity in BA17/18. During the encoding phase of
> Experiment 2, abstract shapes with colored and oriented internal lines
> were presented at fixation. During the retrieval phase, old shapes,
> changed shapes (with the same outline but different colored and oriented
> internal lines), and new shapes were presented at fixation and
> participants made an old-new classification during runs with a specific
> retrieval orientation or a non-specific retrieval orientation.
> Critically, the contrast of old-hits > old-misses during specific
> retrieval orientation produced activity in BA17/18. The results of the
> present experiments support the hypothesis that conscious processing
> during retrieval can occur in BA17/18. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights
> reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 488-492 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000013
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> The motor cortex is causally related to predictive eye movements during action observation
>
> Authors:
> Elsner, C; D'Ausilio, A; Gredeback, G; Falck-Ytter, T; Fadiga, L
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):488-492; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> We examined the hypothesis that predictive gaze during observation of
> other people's actions depends on the activation of corresponding action
> plans in the observer. Using transcranial magnetic stimulation and
> eye-tracking technology we found that stimulation of the motor hand
> area, but not of the leg area, slowed gaze predictive behavior (compared
> to no TMS). This result shows that predictive eye movements to others'
> action goals depend on a somatotopical recruitment of the observer's
> motor system. The study provides direct support for the view that a
> direct matching process implemented in the mirror-neuron system plays a
> functional role for real-time goal prediction. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd.
> All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 493-499 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000014
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Response inhibition triggered by the briefly viewed image of a hand: Behavioural and electrophysiological evidence
>
> Authors:
> Vainio, L; Alen, H; Hiltunen, S; Lehikoinen, K; Lindback, H;
> Patrikainen, A; Paavilainen, P
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):493-499; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Previous research has shown that subliminally presented arrows produce
> negative priming effect in which responses are performed slower when
> primes and targets are calling for the same response than different
> response. This phenomenon has been attributed to self-inhibitory
> mechanisms of response processes. Similar negative priming was recently
> observed when participants responded to the direction of the target
> arrow and the prime was a briefly displayed image of a left or right
> hand. Responses were made slower when the left-right identity of the
> viewed hand was compatible with the responding hand. This was suggested
> to demonstrate that the proposed motor self-inhibition is a general and
> basic functional principle in manual control processes. However, the
> behavioural evidence observed in that study was not capable of showing
> whether the negative priming associated with a briefly displayed hand
> could reflect other inhibitory processes than the motor self-inhibition.
> The present study uses an electrophysiological indicator of automatic
> response priming, the lateralized readiness potential (LRP), to
> investigate whether the negative priming triggered by the identity of
> the viewed hand does indeed reflect motor self-inhibition processes. The
> LRP revealed a pattern of motor activation that was in line with the
> motor self-inhibition hypothesis. Thus, the finding supports the view
> that the self-inhibition mechanisms are not restricted to arrow stimuli
> that are presented subliminally. Rather, they are general sensorimotor
> mechanisms that operate in planning and control of manual actions. (c)
> 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 500-505 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000015
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Interaction between musical emotion and facial expression as measured by event-related potentials
>
> Authors:
> Kamiyama, KS; Abla, D; Iwanaga, K; Okanoya, K
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):500-505; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> We examined the integrative process between emotional facial expressions
> and musical excerpts by using an affective priming paradigm. Happy or
> sad musical stimuli were presented after happy or sad facial images
> during electroencephalography (EEG) recordings. We asked participants to
> judge the affective congruency of the presented face-music pairs. The
> congruency of emotionally congruent pairs was judged more rapidly than
> that of incongruent pairs. In addition, the EEG data showed that
> incongruent musical targets elicited a larger N400 component than
> congruent pairs. Furthermore, these effects occurred in nonmusicians as
> well as musicians. In sum, emotional integrative processing of
> face-music pairs was facilitated in congruent music targets and
> inhibited in incongruent music targets; this process was not
> significantly modulated by individual musical experience. This is the
> first study on musical stimuli primed by facial expressions to
> demonstrate that the N400 component reflects the affective priming
> effect. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 506-519 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000016
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Context and hand posture modulate the neural dynamics of tool-object perception
>
> Authors:
> Natraj, N; Poole, V; Mizelle, JC; Flumini, A; Borghi, AM; Wheaton, LA
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):506-519; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Prior research has linked visual perception of tools with plausible
> motor strategies. Thus, observing a tool activates the putative
> action-stream, including the left posterior parietal cortex. Observing a
> hand functionally grasping a tool involves the inferior frontal cortex.
> However, tool-use movements are performed in a contextual and grasp
> specific manner, rather than relative isolation. Our prior behavioral
> data has demonstrated that the context of tool-use (by pairing the tool
> with different objects) and varying hand grasp postures of the tool can
> interact to modulate subjects' reaction times while evaluating
> tool-object content. Specifically, perceptual judgment was delayed in
> the evaluation of functional tool-object pairings (Correct context) when
> the tool was non-functionally (Manipulative) grasped. Here, we
> hypothesized that this behavioral interference seen with the
> Manipulative posture would be due to increased and extended left
> parietofrontal activity possibly underlying motor simulations when
> resolving action conflict due to this particular grasp at time scales
> relevant to the behavioral data. Further, we hypothesized that this
> neural effect will be restricted to the Correct tool-object context
> wherein action affordances are at a maximum.
> 64-channel electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded from 16
> right-handed subjects while viewing images depicting three classes of
> tool-object contexts: functionally Correct (e.g. coffee pot-coffee mug),
> functionally Incorrect (e.g. coffee pot-marker) and Spatial (coffee
> pot-milk). The Spatial context pairs a tool and object that would not
> functionally match, but may commonly appear in the same scene. These
> three contexts were modified by hand interaction: No Hand, Static Hand
> near the tool, Functional Hand posture and Manipulative Hand posture.
> The Manipulative posture is convenient for relocating a tool but does
> not afford a functional engagement of the tool on the target object.
> Subjects were instructed to visually assess whether the pictures
> displayed correct tool-object associations. EEG data was analyzed in
> time-voltage and time-frequency domains. Overall, Static Hand,
> Functional and Manipulative postures cause early activation (100-400 ms
> post image onset) of parietofrontal areas, to varying intensity in each
> context, when compared to the No Hand control condition. However, when
> context is Correct, only the Manipulative Posture significantly induces
> extended neural responses, predominantly over right parietal and right
> frontal areas [400-600 ms post image onset]. Significant power increase
> was observed in the theta band [4-8 Hz] over the right frontal area,
> [0-500 ms]. In addition, when context is Spatial, Manipulative posture
> alone significantly induces extended neural responses, over bilateral
> parietofrontal and left motor areas [400-600 ms]. Significant power
> decrease occurred primarily in beta bands [12-16, 20-25 Hz] over the
> aforementioned brain areas [400-600 ms].
> Here, we demonstrate that the neural processing of tool-object
> perception is sensitive to several factors. While both Functional and
> Manipulative postures in Correct context engage predominantly an early
> left parietofrontal circuit, the Manipulative posture alone extends the
> neural response and transitions to a late right parietofrontal network.
> This suggests engagement of a right neural system to evaluate action
> affordances when hand posture does not support action (Manipulative).
> Additionally, when tool-use context is ambiguous (Spatial context),
> there is increased bilateral parietofrontal activation and, extended
> neural response for the Manipulative posture. These results point to the
> existence of other networks evaluating tool-object associations when
> motoric affordances are not readily apparent and underlie corresponding
> delayed perceptual judgment in our prior behavioral data wherein
> Manipulative postures had exclusively interfered in judging tool-object
> content. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 520-537 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000017
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Foreign accent syndrome: A multimodal evaluation in the search of neuroscience-driven treatments
>
> Authors:
> Moreno-Torres, I; Berthier, ML; Cid, MD; Green, C; Gutierrez, A;
> Garcia-Casares, N; Walsh, SF; Nabrozidis, A; Sidorova, J; Davila, G;
> Carnero-Pardo, C
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):520-537; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Foreign accent syndrome (FAS) is a rare condition which is placed in the
> mildest end of the spectrum of speech disorders. The impairment, not
> severe enough to elicit phonological errors, is associated with various
> alterations in the fine execution of speech sounds which cause the
> impression of foreignness. There is a growing interest in the study of
> linguistic and paralinguistic components, psychosocial aftermaths, and
> neural basis of FAS, but there are not yet neuroscience-driven
> treatments for this condition. A multimodal evaluation was conducted in
> a single patient with the aim of searching for clues which may assist to
> design neuroscience-driven therapies. The patient was a middle-aged
> bilingual woman who had chronic FAS. She had segmental deficits,
> abnormal production of linguistic and emotional prosody, impaired verbal
> communication, and reduced motivation and social engagement. Magnetic
> resonance imaging showed bilateral small lesions mainly affecting the
> left deep frontal operculum and dorsal anterior insula. Diffusion tensor
> tractography suggested disrupted left deep frontal operculum-anterior
> insula connectivity. Metabolic activity measured with positron emission
> tomography was primarily decreased in key components of networks
> implicated in planning and execution of speech production, cognitive
> control and emotional communication (Brodmann's areas 4/6/9/10/13/25/47,
> basal ganglia, and anterior cerebellar vermis). Compensatory increases
> of metabolic activity were found in cortical areas (left anterior
> cingulate gyrus, left superior temporal gyrus and right prefrontal
> cortex) associated with feedback and focal attention processes critical
> for monitoring and adjustment of verbal utterances. Moreover, bilateral
> structural and functional abnormalities probably interrupted the
> trajectory of the lateral and medial cholinergic pathways causing
> region-specific hypoactivity. The results from this study provide
> targets for further investigation and some clues to design therapeutic
> interventions. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 538-549 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000018
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Musical expertise affects neural bases of letter recognition
>
> Authors:
> Proverbio, AM; Manfredi, M; Zani, A; Adorni, R
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):538-549; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> It is known that early music learning (playing of an instrument)
> modifies functional brain structure (both white and gray matter) and
> connectivity, especially callosal transfer, motor control/coordination
> and auditory processing. We compared visual processing of notes and
> words in 15 professional musicians and 15 controls by recording their
> synchronized bioelectrical activity (ERPs) in response to words and
> notes. We found that musical training in childhood (from age similar to
> 8 years) modifies neural mechanisms of word reading, whatever the
> genetic predisposition, which was unknown. While letter processing was
> strongly left-lateralized in controls, the fusiform (BA37) and inferior
> occipital gyri (BA18) were activated in both hemispheres in musicians
> for both word and music processing. The evidence that the neural
> mechanism of letter processing differed in musicians and controls (being
> absolutely bilateral in musicians) suggests that musical expertise
> modifies the neural mechanisms of letter reading. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd.
> All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 550-556 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000019
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Access to lexical meaning in pitch-flattened Chinese sentences: An fMRI study
>
> Authors:
> Xu, GQ; Zhang, LJ; Shu, H; Wang, XY; Li, P
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):550-556; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Chinese is a tonal language in which variation in pitch is used to
> distinguish word meanings. Thus, in order to understand a word,
> listeners have to extract the pitch patterns in addition to its
> phonemes. Can the correct word meaning still be accessed in sentence
> contexts if pitch patterns of words are altered? If so, how is this
> accomplished? The present study attempts to address such questions with
> event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Native
> speakers of Mandarin Chinese listened to normal and pitch-flattened
> (monotone) speech inside the scanner. The behavioral results indicated
> that they rated monotone sentences as intelligible as normal sentences,
> and performed equally well in a dictation test on the two types of
> sentences. The fMRI results showed that both types of sentences elicited
> similar activation in the left insular, middle and inferior temporal
> gyri, but the monotone sentences elicited greater activation in the left
> planum temporale (PT) compared with normal sentences. These results
> demonstrate that lexical meaning can still be accessed in
> pitch-flattened Chinese sentences, and that this process is realized by
> automatic recovery of the phonological representations of lexical tones
> from the altered tonal patterns. Our findings suggest that the details
> of spoken pitch patterns are not essential for adequate lexical-semantic
> processing during sentence comprehension even in tonal languages like
> Mandarin Chinese, given that listeners can automatically use additional
> neural and cognitive resources to recover distorted tonal patterns in
> sentences. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
> ========================================================================
>
>
> *Pages: 557-571 (Article)
> *View Full Record: http://gateway.webofknowledge.com/gateway/Gateway.cgi?GWVersion=2&SrcAuth=Alerting&SrcApp=Alerting&DestApp=CCC&DestLinkType=FullRecord;KeyUT=CCC:000316158000020
> *Order Full Text [ ]
>
> Title:
> Strategic retrieval and retrieval orientation in reality monitoring studied by event-related potentials (ERPs)
>
> Authors:
> Rosburg, T; Johansson, M; Mecklinger, A
>
> Source:
> *NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA*, 51 (3):557-571; FEB 2013
>
> Abstract:
> Reality monitoring requires the differentiation between perceived and
> imagined events or between our own actions and the actions of others.
> The role of control processes in reality monitoring is yet not fully
> understood. In the current event-related potential (ERP) study, we
> investigated such control processes in the form of retrieval orientation
> and strategic retrieval of nontarget information. At study, complete or
> incomplete object words were presented in sentences. Participants had to
> identify the words as the subject of the sentence (perceive condition)
> or had to complete them upon presentation of a word fragment
> (self-generate condition). The participants' memory accuracy was better
> for generated items than for perceived items, as tested in a subsequent
> memory exclusion task. Comparison of ERPs to new items between the two
> test conditions (i.e. assessing retrieval orientation) showed more
> positive ERPs when generated object names were targeted. Retrieval
> orientation also modulated the early midfrontal old/new effect: Items of
> the self-generate condition elicited this effect irrespective of their
> target/nontarget status, while in response to the less well remembered
> items of the perceive condition it was only found when these items were
> defined as targets. Target retrieval (as reflected in the left-parietal
> old/new effect) occurred in both test conditions, but nontarget
> retrieval was observed only for generated items (when perceived items
> were targeted). Current findings indicate that retrieval orientation can
> modulate familiarity-related processes. The selective occurrence of
> nontarget retrieval for generated items corroborates the concept that
> the ease with which nontarget information can be accessed promotes
> nontarget retrieval. (c) 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
>
>